Older Adults May Achieve Same Cognition as Undergrads

Summary: Improved cognition was seen in older adults who learned multiple new tasks, and the improvements got better as time passed, a new study reports.

Source: UCR

A set of recent studies demonstrates for the first time that learning multiple new tasks carries benefits for cognition long after the learning has been completed.

The finding affirms a long-held assertion of the lead researcher, Rachel Wu, who is an associate professor of psychology at UC Riverside. That is, older adults can learn new tasks and improve their cognition in the process, if they approach learning as a child does.

“Our findings provide evidence that simultaneously learning real-world skills can lead to long-term improvements in cognition during older adulthood,” Wu and her colleagues wrote in a recently published paper in the journal Aging and Mental Health. “Overall, our findings promote the benefits of lifelong learning, namely, to improve cognitive abilities in older adulthood.”

One year after they learned new skills, the older research subjects tested higher for certain cognitive tasks than prior to the learning. Consistently, the scores for cognitive functions increased on average by at least two to three times, sometimes more.

The first study had six participants, the second study, 27. The median age of the study subjects was 66 and 69 years old, respectively. To qualify for the study, participants had to be at least 55 years old, fluent in English, have normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and have no prior diagnosis of a cognitive impairment.

Participants in this study completed classwork and homework for approximately 15 hours a week for three months, time constraints Wu said limited the number of eligible participants.

For both studies, the participants learned at least three new skills, such as Spanish, using an iPad, photography, painting, and music composition over three months in a UC Riverside classroom for older adults. Cognitive tests were administered in a research lab before the start of the classes, halfway through the classes, and after three months of classes. There were then follow-up tests at three months, six months, and one year after the end of the classes.

 “The primary goal of the follow-up assessments was to determine if gains in cognitive abilities… would continue up to one year after the intervention,” the authors wrote.

The cognitive measures included attention, inhibition, and short-term memory, which requires remembering small amounts of information needed to perform tasks, such as recalling a phone number or words from a list.

The overall cognitive scores at three months, six months, and one year after the intervention were significantly higher than before the intervention, more than three times higher by many measures. In fact, the more time that passed after the learning had ceased, the higher the scores grew. 

This shows an older man
One year after they learned new skills, the older research subjects tested higher for certain cognitive tasks than prior to the learning. Image is in the public domain

“Remarkably, the cognitive scores increased to levels similar to undergraduates taking the same cognitive tests for the first time,” Wu said. “Our finding of continuous cognitive growth in older adulthood is unique because most studies show only maintenance of cognitive abilities or cognitive decline over time.”

The key to the difference, Wu surmises, is learning multiple tasks simultaneously in an encouraging environment, similar to what children experience.

“The time and energy commitment to do so was similar to a full undergraduate course load,” Wu and her colleagues wrote in the paper, titled “One-year cognitive outcomes from a multiple real-world skill learning intervention with older adults.”

For Wu, it is further affirmation of her past research, which demonstrated that older adults can learn by emulating the learning behaviors of children. Among other things, it means older adults must approach learning with an open mind, unafraid of criticism and failure, receptive to instruction, willing to learn multiple tasks at once, and with a belief they can improve with effort.

Along with Wu, authors of the paper include Leah Ferguson, Debaleena Sain, and Esra Kürüm, all of UCR; Carla M. Strickland-Hughes of University of the Pacific, and George W. Rebok, of Johns Hopkins University. 

About this cognition and aging research news

Author: John Warren
Source: UCR
Contact: John Warren – UCR
Image: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Open access.
One-year cognitive outcomes from a multiple real-world skill learning intervention with older adults” by Rachel Wu et al. Aging & Mental Health


Abstract

One-year cognitive outcomes from a multiple real-world skill learning intervention with older adults

Objectives

Novel skill learning has been shown to have cognitive benefits in the short-term (up to a few months). Two studies expanded on prior research by investigating whether learning multiple novel real-world skills simultaneously (e.g. Spanish, drawing, music composition), for a minimum of six hours a week, would yield 1-year cognitive gains.

Method

Following a 3-month multi-skill learning intervention, Study 1 (N = 6, Mage = 66 years, SDage = 6.41) and Study 2 (N = 27, Mage = 69 years, SDage = 7.12) participants completed follow-up cognitive assessments 3 months, 6 months, and one year after the intervention period. Cognitive assessments tested executive function (working memory and cognitive control) and verbal episodic memory.

Results

Linear mixed-effects models revealed improvements in multiple cognitive outcomes from before the intervention to the follow-up timepoints. Specifically, executive function increased from pre-test to the 1-year follow-up for both studies (an effect driven mostly by cognitive control scores).

Discussion

Our findings provide evidence that simultaneously learning real-world skills can lead to long-term improvements in cognition during older adulthood. Future work with diverse samples could investigate individual differences in gains. Overall, our findings promote the benefits of lifelong learning, namely, to improve cognitive abilities in older adulthood.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. Thankyou for your research I am 77 and I seem to have been able to learn far more than I could have imagined after my son gave me a present of a smart phone and wouldn’t teach me to use it I was a technophobe but I eventually learnt now I research extensively I’m partly dyslexic however i seem to have got around that problem to learn my interests more than I was able to before I feel that your research will change the of old age I feel confident and positive that you can change the world thankyou 😊

  2. I am 71 and just completed a university short course in a field totally unknown to me. Got 83%- 4th highest in a class of 54 students. Obtained a Masters degree in law at age 55. Age was never a factor – in fact, life experience enriched my assignments and ability to debate matters.

Comments are closed.